We
like to think of ourselves as rational creatures. We watch our backs, weigh the
odds, pack an umbrella. But both neuroscience and social science suggest that
we are more optimistic than realistic. On average, we expect things to turn out
better than they wind up being. People hugely underestimate their chances of
getting divorced, losing their job or being diagnosed with cancer; expect their
children to be extraordinarily gifted; envision themselves achieving more than
their peers; and overestimate their likely life span (sometimes by 20 years or
more).
The
belief that the future will be much better than the past and present is known
as the optimism bias. It abides in every race, region and socioeconomic
bracket. Schoolchildren playing when-I-grow-up are rampant optimists, but so
are grown-ups: a 2005 study found that adults over 60 are just as likely to see
the glass half full as young adults.
You
might expect optimism to erode under the tide of news about violent conflicts,
high unemployment, tornadoes and floods and all the threats and failures that
shape human life. Collectively we can grow pessimistic – about the direction of
our country or the ability of our leaders to improve education and reduce
crime. But private optimism, about our personal future, remains incredibly
resilient. A survey conducted in 2007 found that while 70% thought families in
general were less successful than in their parents' day, 76% of respondents
were optimistic about the future of their own family.
Overly
positive assumptions can lead to disastrous miscalculations – make us less
likely to get health checkups, apply sunscreen or open a savings account, and
more likely to bet the farm on a bad investment. But the bias also protects and
inspires us: it keeps us moving forward rather than to the nearest high-rise
ledge. Without optimism, our ancestors might never have ventured far from their
tribes and we might all be cave dwellers, still huddled together and dreaming
of light and heat.
To
make progress, we need to be able to imagine alternative realities – better
ones – and we need to believe that we can achieve them. Such faith helps
motivate us to pursue our goals. Optimists in general work longer hours and
tend to earn more. Economists at Duke University found that optimists even save
more. And although they are not less likely to divorce, they are more likely to
remarry – an act that is, as Samuel Johnson wrote, the triumph of hope over
experience.
Even
if that better future is often an illusion, optimism has clear benefits in the
present. Hope keeps our minds at ease, lowers stress and improves physical
health. Researchers studying heart-disease patients found that optimists were
more likely than non-optimistic patients to take vitamins, eat low-fat diets
and exercise, thereby reducing their overall coronary risk. A study of cancer
patients revealed that pessimistic patients under 60 were more likely to die
within eight months than non-pessimistic patients of the same initial health,
status and age.
In
fact, a growing body of scientific evidence points to the conclusion that
optimism may be hardwired by evolution into the human brain. The science of
optimism, once scorned as an intellectually suspect province of pep rallies and
smiley faces, is opening a new window on the workings of human consciousness.
What it shows could fuel a revolution in psychology, as the field comes to
grips with accumulating evidence that our brains aren't just stamped by the
past. They are constantly being shaped by the future.
Hardwired
for hope?
I
would have liked to tell you that my work on optimism grew out of a keen
interest in the positive side of human nature. The reality is that I stumbled
onto the brain's innate optimism by accident. After living through 9/11, in New
York City, I had set out to investigate people's memories of the terrorist
attacks. I was intrigued by the fact that people felt their memories were as
accurate as a videotape, while often they were filled with errors. A survey
conducted around the country showed that 11 months after the attacks,
individuals' recollections of their experience that day were consistent with
their initial accounts (given in September 2011) only 63% of the time. They were
also poor at remembering details of the event, such as the names of the airline
carriers. Where did these mistakes in memory come from?
Scientists
who study memory proposed an intriguing answer: memories are susceptible to
inaccuracies partly because the neural system responsible for remembering
episodes from our past might not have evolved for memory alone. Rather, the
core function of the memory system could in fact be to imagine the future – to
enable us to prepare for what has yet to come. The system is not designed to
perfectly replay past events, the researchers claimed. It is designed to
flexibly construct future scenarios in our minds. As a result, memory also ends
up being a reconstructive process, and occasionally, details are deleted and
others inserted.
To
test this, I decided to record the brain activity of volunteers while they
imagined future events – not events on the scale of 9/11, but events in their
everyday lives – and compare those results with the pattern I observed when the
same individuals recalled past events. But something unexpected occurred. Once
people started imagining the future, even the most banal life events seemed to
take a dramatic turn for the better. Mundane scenes brightened with upbeat
details as if polished by a Hollywood script doctor. You might think that
imagining a future haircut would be pretty dull. Not at all. Here is what one
of my participants pictured: "I was getting my hair cut to donate to Locks
of Love [a charity that fashions wigs for young cancer patients]. It had taken
me years to grow it out, and my friends were all there to help celebrate. We
went to my favourite hair place in Brooklyn and then went to lunch at our
favourite restaurant."
I
asked another participant to imagine a plane ride. "I imagined the takeoff
– my favourite! – and then the eight-hour-long nap in between and then finally
landing in Krakow and clapping the pilot for providing the safe voyage,"
she responded. No tarmac delays, no screaming babies. The world, only a year or
two into the future, was a wonderful place to live in.
If
all our participants insisted on thinking positively when it came to what lay
in store for them personally, what does that tell us about how our brains are
wired? Is the human tendency for optimism a consequence of the architecture of
our brains?
The
Human time machine
To
think positively about our prospects, we must first be able to imagine
ourselves in the future. Optimism starts with what may be the most
extraordinary of human talents: mental time travel, the ability to move back
and forth through time and space in one's mind. Although most of us take this
ability for granted, our capacity to envision a different time and place is in
fact critical to our survival.
It is
easy to see why cognitive time travel was naturally selected for over the
course of evolution. It allows us to plan ahead, to save food and resources for
times of scarcity and to endure hard work in anticipation of a future reward.
It also lets us forecast how our current behaviour may influence future
generations. If we were not able to picture the world in a hundred years or
more, would we be concerned with global warming? Would we attempt to live
healthily? Would we have children?
While
mental time travel has clear survival advantages, conscious foresight came to
humans at an enormous price – the understanding that somewhere in the future,
death awaits. Ajit Varki, a biologist at the University of California, San
Diego, argues that the awareness of mortality on its own would have led evolution
to a dead end. The despair would have interfered with our daily function,
bringing the activities needed for survival to a stop. The only way conscious
mental time travel could have arisen over the course of evolution is if it
emerged together with irrational optimism. Knowledge of death had to emerge
side by side with the persistent ability to picture a bright future.
The
capacity to envision the future relies partly on the hippocampus, a brain
structure that is crucial to memory. Patients with damage to their hippocampus
are unable to recollect the past, but they are also unable to construct
detailed images of future scenarios. They appear to be stuck in time. The rest
of us constantly move back and forth in time; we might think of a conversation
we had with our spouse yesterday and then immediately of our dinner plans for
later tonight.
But
the brain doesn't travel in time in a random fashion. It tends to engage in
specific types of thoughts. We consider how well our kids will do in life, how
we will obtain that sought-after job, afford that house on the hill and find
perfect love. We imagine our team winning the crucial game, look forward to an
enjoyable night on the town or picture a winning streak at the blackjack table.
We also worry about losing loved ones, failing at our job or dying in a
terrible plane crash – but research shows that most of us spend less time
mulling over negative outcomes than we do over positive ones. When we do
contemplate defeat and heartache, we tend to focus on how these can be avoided.
Findings
from a study I conducted a few years ago with prominent neuroscientist
Elizabeth Phelps suggest that directing our thoughts of the future toward the
positive is a result of our frontal cortex's communicating with subcortical regions
deep in our brain. The frontal cortex, a large area behind the forehead, is the
most recently evolved part of the brain. It is larger in humans than in other
primates and is critical for many complex human functions such as language and
goal setting. Using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanner, we
recorded brain activity in volunteers as they imagined specific events that
might occur to them in the future. Some of the events that I asked them to
imagine were desirable (a great date or winning a large sum of money), and some
were undesirable (losing a wallet, ending a romantic relationship). The
volunteers reported that their images of sought-after events were richer and
more vivid than those of unwanted events.
This
matched the enhanced activity we observed in two critical regions of the brain:
the amygdala, a small structure deep in the brain that is central to the
processing of emotion, and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), an
area of the frontal cortex that modulates emotion and motivation. The rACC acts
like a traffic conductor, enhancing the flow of positive emotions and
associations. The more optimistic a person was, the higher the activity in
these regions was while imagining positive future events (relative to negative
ones) and the stronger the connectivity between the two structures.
The
findings were particularly fascinating because these precise regions – the
amygdala and the rACC – show abnormal activity in depressed individuals. While
healthy people expect the future to be slightly better than it ends up being,
people with severe depression tend to be pessimistically biased: they expect
things to be worse than they end up being. People with mild depression are
relatively accurate when predicting future events. They see the world as it is.
In other words, in the absence of a neural mechanism that generates unrealistic
optimism, it is possible all humans would be mildly depressed.
Can
optimism change reality?
The
problem with pessimistic expectations, such as those of the clinically
depressed, is that they have the power to alter the future; negative
expectations shape outcomes in a negative way. How do expectations change
reality?
To
answer this question my colleague, cognitive neuroscientist Sara Bengtsson, devised
an experiment in which she manipulated positive and negative expectations of
students while their brains were scanned and tested their performance on
cognitive tasks. To induce expectations of success, she primed college students
with words such as smart, intelligent and clever just before asking them to
perform a test. To induce expectations of failure, she primed them with words
like stupid and ignorant. The students performed better after being primed with
an affirmative message.
Examining
the brain-imaging data, Bengtsson found that the students' brains responded
differently to the mistakes they made depending on whether they were primed
with the word clever or the word stupid. When the mistake followed positive
words, she observed enhanced activity in the anterior medial part of the
prefrontal cortex (a region that is involved in self-reflection and
recollection). However, when the participants were primed with the word stupid,
there was no heightened activity after a wrong answer. It appears that after
being primed with the word stupid, the brain expected to do poorly and did not
show signs of surprise or conflict when it made an error.
A
brain that doesn't expect good results lacks a signal telling it, "Take
notice – wrong answer!" These brains will fail to learn from their
mistakes and are less likely to improve over time. Expectations become
self-fulfilling by altering our performance and actions, which ultimately
affects what happens in the future. Often, however, expectations simply transform
the way we perceive the world without altering reality itself. Let me give you
an example. While writing these lines, my friend calls. He is at Heathrow
waiting to get on a plane to Austria for a skiing holiday. His plane has been
delayed for three hours already, because of snowstorms at his destination.
"I guess this is both a good and bad thing," he says.
Waiting
at the airport is not pleasant, but he quickly concludes that snow today means
better skiing conditions tomorrow. His brain works to match the unexpected
misfortune of being stuck at the airport to its eager anticipation of a fun
getaway.
A
cancelled flight is hardly tragic, but even when the incidents that befall us
are the type of horrific events we never expected to encounter, we automatically
seek evidence confirming that our misfortune is a blessing in disguise. No, we
did not anticipate losing our job, being ill or getting a divorce, but when
these incidents occur, we search for the upside. These experiences mature us,
we think. They may lead to more fulfilling jobs and stable relationships in the
future. Interpreting a misfortune in this way allows us to conclude that our
sunny expectations were correct after all – things did work out for the best.
The
role of the caudate nucleus
How
do we find the silver lining in storm clouds? To answer that, my colleagues –
renowned neuroscientist Ray Dolan and neurologist Tamara Shiner – and I
instructed volunteers in the fMRI scanner to visualise a range of medical
conditions, from broken bones to Alzheimer's, and rate how bad they imagined
these conditions to be. Then we asked them: If you had to endure one of the
following, which would you rather have – a broken leg or a broken arm?
Heartburn or asthma? Finally, they rated all the conditions again. Minutes
after choosing one particular illness out of many, the volunteers suddenly
found that the chosen illness was less intimidating. A broken leg, for example,
may have been thought of as "terrible" before choosing it over some
other malady. However, after choosing it, the subject would find a silver
lining: "With a broken leg, I will be able to lie in bed watching TV,
guilt-free."
In
our study, we also found that people perceived adverse events more positively
if they had experienced them in the past. Recording brain activity while these
reappraisals took place revealed that highlighting the positive within the
negative involves, once again, a tête-à-tête between the frontal cortex and
subcortical regions processing emotional value. While contemplating a mishap,
like a broken leg, activity in the rACC modulated signals in a region called
the striatum that conveyed the good and bad of the event in question – biasing
activity in a positive direction.
It
seems that our brain possesses the philosopher's stone that enables us to turn
lead into gold and helps us bounce back to normal levels of wellbeing. It is
wired to place high value on the events we encounter and put faith in its own
decisions. This is true not only when forced to choose between two adverse
options (such as selecting between two courses of medical treatment) but also
when we are selecting between desirable alternatives. Imagine you need to pick
between two equally attractive job offers. Making a decision may be a tiring,
difficult ordeal, but once you make up your mind, something miraculous happens.
Suddenly – if you are like most people – you view the chosen offer as better
than you did before and conclude that the other option was not that great after
all. According to social psychologist Leon Festinger, we re-evaluate the
options post-choice to reduce the tension that arises from making a difficult
decision between equally desirable options.
In a
brain-imaging study I conducted with Ray Dolan and Benedetto De Martino in
2009, we asked subjects to imagine going on vacation to 80 different
destinations and rate how happy they thought they would be in each place. We
then asked them to select one destination from two choices that they had rated
exactly the same. Would you choose Paris over Brazil? Finally, we asked them to
imagine and rate all the destinations again. Seconds after picking between two
destinations, people rated their selected destination higher than before and
rated the discarded choice lower than before.
The
brain-imaging data revealed that these changes were happening in the caudate
nucleus, a cluster of nerve cells that is part of the striatum. The caudate has
been shown to process rewards and signal their expectation. If we believe we
are about to be given a paycheck or eat a scrumptious chocolate cake, the
caudate acts as an announcer broadcasting to other parts of the brain, "Be
ready for something good." After we receive the reward, the value is
quickly updated. If there is a bonus in the paycheck, this higher value will be
reflected in striatal activity. If the cake is disappointing, the decreased
value will be tracked so that next time our expectations will be lower.
In
our experiment, after a decision was made between two destinations, the caudate
nucleus rapidly updated its signal. Before choosing, it might signal
"thinking of something great" while imagining both Greece and
Thailand. But after choosing Greece, it now broadcast "thinking of
something remarkable!" for Greece and merely "thinking of something
good" for Thailand.
True,
sometimes we regret our decisions; our choices can turn out to be
disappointing. But on balance, when you make a decision – even if it is a
hypothetical choice – you will value it more and expect it to bring you
pleasure.
This
affirmation of our decisions helps us derive heightened pleasure from choices
that might actually be neutral. Without this, our lives might well be filled
with second-guessing. Have we done the right thing? Should we change our mind?
We would find ourselves stuck, overcome by indecision and unable to move
forward.
The
puzzle of optimism
While
the past few years have seen important advances in the neuroscience of
optimism, one enduring puzzle remained. How is it that people maintain this
rosy bias even when information challenging our upbeat forecasts is so readily
available? Only recently have we been able to decipher this mystery, by
scanning the brains of people as they process both positive and negative
information about the future. The findings are striking: when people learn,
their neurons faithfully encode desirable information that can enhance optimism
but fail at incorporating unexpectedly undesirable information. When we hear a
success story like Mark Zuckerberg's, our brains take note of the possibility
that we too may become immensely rich one day. But hearing that the odds of
divorce are almost one in two tends not to make us think that our own marriages
may be destined to fail.
Why
would our brains be wired in this way? It is tempting to speculate that optimism
was selected by evolution precisely because, on balance, positive expectations
enhance the odds of survival. Research findings that optimists live longer and
are healthier, plus the fact that most humans display optimistic biases – and
emerging data that optimism is linked to specific genes – all strongly support
this hypothesis. Yet optimism is also irrational and can lead to unwanted
outcomes. The question then is, How can we remain hopeful – benefiting from the
fruits of optimism – while at the same time guarding ourselves from its
pitfalls?
I
believe knowledge is key. We are not born with an innate understanding of our
biases. The brain's illusions have to be identified by careful scientific
observation and controlled experiments and then communicated to the rest of us.
Once we are made aware of our optimistic illusions, we can act to protect
ourselves. The good news is that awareness rarely shatters the illusion. The
glass remains half full. It is possible, then, to strike a balance, to believe
we will stay healthy, but get medical insurance anyway; to be certain the sun
will shine, but grab an umbrella on our way out — just in case.
Tali
Sharot is a research fellow at University College London's Wellcome Trust
Centre for Neuroimaging
©
2011 Tali Sharot
No comments:
Post a Comment